
This article may require cleanup to meet Wikipedia's quality standards. The specific problem is: Some sections remain fragmented or weakly integrated (e.g. Sensation, Needs, Perception). Further editing is needed to improve cohesion.(March 2025) |
According to the APA Dictionary of Psychology, a feeling is "a self-contained phenomenal experience"; feelings are "subjective, evaluative, and independent of the sensations, thoughts, or images evoking them". The term feeling is closely related to, but not the same as, emotion. Feeling may, for instance, refer to the conscious subjective experience of emotions. The study of subjective experiences is called phenomenology. Psychotherapy generally involves a therapist helping a client understand, articulate, and learn to effectively regulate the client's own feelings, and ultimately to take responsibility for the client's experience of the world. Feelings are sometimes held to be characteristic of embodied consciousness.
The English noun feelings may generally refer to any degree of subjectivity in perception or sensation. However, feelings often refer to an individual sense of well-being (perhaps of wholeness, safety, or being loved). Feelings have a semantic field extending from the individual and spiritual to the social and political. The word feeling may refer to any of a number of psychological characteristics of experience, or even to reflect the entire inner life of the individual (see mood.) As self-contained phenomenal experiences, evoked by sensations and perceptions, feelings can strongly influence the character of a person's subjective reality. Feelings can sometimes harbor bias or otherwise distort veridical perception, in particular through projection, wishful thinking, among many other such effects.
Feeling may also describe the senses, such as the physical sensation of touch.
Definitions and distinctions
In psychology and philosophy, feeling is commonly defined as the subjective experience of emotion or sensation. Although the terms feeling, emotion, affect, and mood are sometimes used interchangeably in everyday language, they have distinct meanings in academic contexts.
According to psychologist Carroll Izard, feelings are best understood as the conscious experience of emotion, arising when an affective state reaches awareness. William James similarly proposed that feelings result from the perception of bodily changes in response to external stimuli, thus forming part of the emotional process. More recently, affective neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp emphasized the role of subcortical brain systems in generating core affects that underlie both feelings and emotions.
Lisa Feldman Barrett argues that feelings are constructed mental representations, emerging from the brain's interpretation of interoceptive signals combined with prior experience and cultural concepts. In this view, feelings are not innate, discrete entities but are formed through predictive processes. In philosophical psychology, particularly in the work of Carl Jung, feeling is considered one of the four primary functions of consciousness, alongside thinking, sensation, and intuition. Unlike emotions, which are often reactive, Jung defined feeling as a rational function that evaluates and assigns value.
Feeling also differs from sensation: while sensation refers to raw sensory input (such as touch, heat, or pain), feelings involve evaluative or affective judgements about those sensations or experiences. Similarly, moods are typically more diffuse and long-lasting affective states, while feelings tend to be more transient and directly tied to particular events or thoughts. These distinctions are foundational in fields such as affective science, philosophy of mind, and cognitive psychology, where the term feeling plays a central role in understanding consciousness, subjectivity, and emotional life.
Neuroscientist Antonio Damasio distinguishes between emotions and feelings: Emotions are mental images (i.e. representing either internal or external states of reality) and the bodily changes accompanying them, whereas feelings are the perception of bodily changes. In other words, emotions contain a subjective element and a third-person observable element, whereas feelings are subjective and private.[10]
Historical and philosophical views
The English word feeling derives from Old English fēlan, meaning "to touch or perceive through the senses", and later acquired the meaning of internal emotional experience. Early philosophical and psychological approaches to feeling laid the foundation for later distinctions between affect, emotion, and cognition. These perspectives treated feeling not merely as emotional fluctuation but as a central dimension of human experience, evaluative thought, and even moral judgement.
The systematic study of affect and feeling (gefühl) in psychology began in the late 19th century with the work of Wilhelm Wundt, often considered the founder of experimental psychology. Wundt proposed that affective experience could be described along three dimensions: pleasant–unpleasant, arousing–subduing, and strain–relaxation. These affective dimensions laid the groundwork for later theories of emotional valence and arousal.
A decade later, William James proposed a physiological theory of emotion in which feelings are the perception of bodily changes caused by external stimuli. In his classic 1884 essay, he wrote: "we feel sorry because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble", arguing that feeling follows bodily reaction rather than preceding it.
In the early 20th century, Carl Jung developed a typology in which feeling was one of the four fundamental functions of consciousness, alongside thinking, sensation, and intuition. Unlike emotion, which he considered reactive and affective, Jung defined feeling as a rational function that judges and evaluates rather than perceives or reacts. In this view, feeling can be used to assign value or make decisions, independent of sensory experience.
Meanwhile, in phenomenological philosophy, Max Scheler emphasized that feeling is a unique mode of access to values. Rather than viewing feelings as subjective or irrational, Scheler argued that they are intentional acts that disclose the worth of things—what he called "value-feelings" (wertgefühle). This idea positioned feeling not only as affective but also as epistemological.
Contemporary philosopher Martha Nussbaum has continued the philosophical development of feeling by arguing that emotions are a form of evaluative judgement. Drawing from classical philosophy, she suggests that emotions are not opposed to rationality but are instead shaped by beliefs about what is valuable and significant. In her account, feelings are deeply intertwined with ethical reasoning and human flourishing.
Cross-cultural and contemplative views
Conceptions of feeling vary widely across cultures and philosophical traditions. In many non-Western frameworks, feeling is not merely a passive or internal state, but a central mode of perceiving, valuing, and engaging with the world.
In Buddhist psychology, particularly in the Abhidharma and Mahayana traditions, feelings (Sanskrit: vedanā) are classified into three primary types: pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral. These are not emotions per se, but rather the basic hedonic tone that arises with every moment of experience. The recognition and mindfulness of feeling tones is foundational in the Satipatthana system of meditation, particularly in the practice of vedanānupassanā—the contemplation of feelings as transient phenomena.
In Tibetan Buddhist systems, these basic feeling tones are further elaborated into a structured typology known as the 51 mental factors, which include both innate and cultivated emotional and cognitive states. Feeling in this context is interwoven with attentional processes, ethical evaluation, and the potential for insight. These frameworks regard feeling as a dynamic event in the continuum of mind, with implications for both enlightenment and suffering.
Hindu philosophy, particularly in the context of rasa theory, offers another model in which feelings are treated not as inner states alone but as aesthetic-emotional essences (rasa) that are evoked and shared through performance, poetry, and religious experience. Classical Indian aesthetics identifies nine primary rasas, such as love (śṛṅgāra), sorrow (karuṇa), and wonder (adbhuta), each associated with a specific emotional flavor that is both individually felt and socially mediated.
Scientific theories and models
A number of experiments have been conducted in the study of social and psychological affective preferences (i.e., what people like or dislike). Specific research has been done on preferences, attitudes, impression formation, and decision-making. This research contrasts findings with recognition memory (old-new judgments), allowing researchers to demonstrate reliable distinctions between the two. Affect-based judgments and cognitive processes have been examined with noted differences indicated. Some argue affect and cognition are under the control of separate and partially independent systems that can influence each other in a variety of ways. Both affect and cognition may constitute independent sources of effects within systems of information processing. Others suggest emotion is a result of an anticipated, experienced, or imagined outcome of an adaptational transaction between organism and environment, therefore cognitive appraisal processes are keys to the development and expression of an emotion.
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (March 2025) |
Sensations
Sensation occurs when sense organs collect various stimuli (such as a sound or smell) for transduction, meaning transformation into a form that can be understood by the nervous system.

Interoception
Gut

A gut feeling, or gut reaction, is a visceral emotional reaction to something. It may be negative, such as a feeling of uneasiness, or positive, such as a feeling of trust. Gut feelings are generally regarded as not modulated by conscious thought, but sometimes as a feature of intuition rather than rationality. The idea that emotions are experienced in the gut has a long historical legacy, and many nineteenth-century doctors considered the origins of mental illness to derive from the intestines.[22]
The phrase "gut feeling" may also be used as a shorthand term for an individual's "common sense" perception of what is considered "the right thing to do", such as helping an injured passerby, avoiding dark alleys and generally acting in accordance with instinctive feelings about a given situation. It can also refer to simple common knowledge phrases which are true no matter when said, such as "Fire is hot", or to ideas that an individual intuitively regards as true (see "truthiness" for examples).
Heart
The heart has a collection of ganglia that is called the "intrinsic cardiac nervous system". The feelings of affiliation, love, attachment, anger, hurt are usually associated with the heart, especially the feeling of love.
Needs
A need is something required to sustain a healthy life (e.g. air, water, food). A (need) deficiency causes a clear adverse outcome: a dysfunction or death. Abraham H. Maslow, pointed out that satisfying (i.e., gratification of) a need, is just as important as deprivation (i.e., motivation to satisfy), for it releases the focus of the satisfied need, to other emergent needs.
Motivation
Motivation is what explains why people or animals initiate, continue or terminate a certain behavior at a particular time. Motivational states are commonly understood as forces acting within the agent that create a disposition to engage in goal-directed behavior. It is often held that different mental states compete with each other and that only the strongest state determines behavior.
Valence
Valence tells organisms (e.g., humans) how well or how bad an organism is doing (in relation to the environment), for meeting the organism's needs.
Perception
Feelings of certainty
The way that we see other people express their emotions or feelings determines how we respond. The way an individual responds to a situation is based on feeling rules. If an individual is uninformed about a situation the way they respond would be in a completely different demeanor than if they were informed about a situation. For example, if a tragic event had occurred and they had knowledge of it, their response would be sympathetic to that situation. If they had no knowledge of the situation, then their response may be indifference. A lack of knowledge or information about an event can shape the way an individual sees things and the way they respond.
Timothy D. Wilson, a psychology professor, tested this theory of the feeling of uncertainty along with his colleague Yoav Bar-Anan, a social psychologist. Wilson and Bar-Anan found that the more uncertain or unclear an individual is about a situation, the more invested they are. Since an individual does not know the background or the ending of a story they are constantly replaying an event in their mind which is causing them to have mixed feelings of happiness, sadness, excitement, and et cetera. If there is any difference between feelings and emotions, the feeling of uncertainty is less sure than the emotion of ambivalence: the former is precarious, the latter is not yet acted upon or decided upon.
The neurologist Robert Burton, writes in his book On Being Certain, that feelings of certainty may stem from involuntary mental sensations, much like emotions or perceptual recognition (another example might be the tip of the tongue phenomenon).
Individuals in society want to know every detail about something in hopes to maximize the feeling for that moment, but Wilson found that feeling uncertain can lead to something being more enjoyable because it has a sense of mystery. In fact, the feeling of not knowing can lead them to constantly think and feel about what could have been.
Sense of agency and sense of ownership
This section is empty. You can help by adding to it. (March 2025) |
Feelings about feelings

Individuals in society predict that something will give them a certain desired outcome or feeling. Indulging in what one might have thought would've made them happy or excited might only cause a temporary thrill, or it might result in the opposite of what was expected and wanted. Events and experiences are done and relived to satisfy one's feelings.[citation needed]
Details and information about the past are used to make decisions, as past experiences of feelings tend to influence current decision-making, how people will feel in the future, and if they want to feel that way again. Gilbert and Wilson conducted a study to show how pleased a person would feel if they purchased flowers for themselves for no specific reason (birthday, anniversary, or promotion etc.) and how long they thought that feeling would last. People who had no experience of purchasing flowers for themselves and those who had experienced buying flowers for themselves were tested. Results showed that those who had purchased flowers in the past for themselves felt happier and that feeling lasted longer for them than for a person who had never experienced purchasing flowers for themselves.
Arlie Russell Hochschild, a sociologist, depicted two accounts of emotion. The organismic emotion is the outburst of emotions and feelings. In organismic emotion, emotions/feelings are instantly expressed. Social and other factors do not influence how the emotion is perceived, so these factors have no control on how or if the emotion is suppressed or expressed. In interactive emotion, emotions and feelings are controlled. The individual is constantly considering how to react or what to suppress. In interactive emotion, unlike in organismic emotion, the individual is aware of their decision on how they feel and how they show it.
Erving Goffman, a sociologist and writer, compared how actors withheld their emotions to the everyday individual. Like actors, individuals can control how emotions are expressed, but they cannot control their inner emotions or feelings. Inner feelings can only be suppressed in order to achieve the expression one wants people to see on the outside. Goffman explains that emotions and emotional experience are an ongoing thing that an individual is consciously and actively working through. Individuals want to conform to society with their inner and outer feelings.
Anger, happiness, joy, stress, and excitement are some of the feelings that can be experienced in life. In response to these emotions, our bodies react as well. For example, nervousness can lead to the sensation of having "knots in the stomach" or "butterflies in the stomach".
Self-harm
Negative feelings can lead to harm. When an individual is dealing with an overwhelming amount of stress and problems in their lives, there is the possibility that they might consider self-harm. When one is in a good state of feeling, they never want it to end; conversely, when someone is in a bad state of mind, they want that feeling to disappear. Inflicting harm or pain to oneself is sometimes the answer for many individuals because they want something to keep their mind off the real problem. These individuals cut, stab, and starve themselves in an effort to feel something other than what they currently feel, as they believe the pain to be not as bad as their actual problem. Distraction is not the only reason why many individuals choose to inflict self-harm. Some people inflict self-harm to punish themselves for feeling a certain way. Other psychological factors could be low self-esteem, the need to be perfect, or social anxiety.
See also
- Affective science
- Alexithymia
- Cognitive neuroscience
- Hard problem of consciousness
- Mind–body problem
- Qualia
Notes
- In German psychology, the term gefühl (meaning “feeling”) has long been used to refer to subjective emotional experience, and it continues to play a role in German-language affective science and phenomenology.
References
- "APA Dictionary of Psychology". dictionary.apa.org. Retrieved 2022-03-24.
- VandenBos, Gary (2006) APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
- Solms, Mark (2021). The hidden spring : a journey to the source of consciousness. London. ISBN 978-1-78816-283-8. OCLC 1190847187.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - Izard (1977).
- James (1884).
- Panksepp (1998).
- Barrett (2017).
- Jung (1971); Sharp (1987).
- Panksepp (1998); Ekkekakis (2013); Lewis, Haviland-Jones & Barrett (2016).
- Damasio (1994), p. [page needed]; Damasio (1999), p. [page needed].
- Harre & Parrott (1996).
- Wundt (1897).
- Sharp (1987).
- Scheler (1973).
- Nussbaum (2001).
- Tsoknyi Rinpoche (1998).
- Analayo (2003).
- Tsoknyi Rinpoche (1998); Berzin (2006).
- Gerow (1982).
- Zajonc (1980).
- Lazarus (1982).
- Mathias & Moore (2018), p. [page needed].
- Fedele & Brand (2020).
- Maslow (1970), p. 38.
- Wasserman & Wasserman (2020).
- Hochschild (2003).
- Bar-Anan, Wilson & Gilbert (2009).
- Burton (2008).
- Horne & Csipke (2009).
- Wood & Bettman (2007).
- Hochschild (1979).
- Hawton, Saunders & O'Connor (2012).
Works cited
- Analayo (2003). Satipaṭṭhāna: The Direct Path to Realization. Windhorse Publications.
- Bar-Anan, Y.; Wilson, T. D.; Gilbert, D. T. (2009). "The feeling of uncertainty intensifies affective reactions". Emotion. 9 (1): 123–7. doi:10.1037/a0014607. PMID 19186925. S2CID 10179263.
- Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
- Berzin, Alexander (2006). "Primary Minds and the 51 Mental Factors". Study Buddhism. Retrieved 2025-03-31.
- Burton, R. A. (2008). On Being Certain: Believing You Are Right Even When You're Not. St. Martin's Press. ISBN 978-0-312-35920-1.
- Damasio, Antonio (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam. ISBN 0-399-13894-3.
- Damasio, Antonio (1999). The Feeling of what Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace. ISBN 978-0-15-601075-7.
- Ekkekakis, Panteleimon (2013). The Measurement of Affect, Mood, and Emotion: A Guide for Health-Behavioral Research. Cambridge University Press.
- Fedele, Laura; Brand, Thomas (December 2020). "The Intrinsic Cardiac Nervous System and Its Role in Cardiac Pacemaking and Conduction". Journal of Cardiovascular Development and Disease. 7 (4): 54. doi:10.3390/jcdd7040054. PMC 7712215. PMID 33255284.
- Gerow, Edwin (1982). "Rasa as a Category of Literary Criticism". In Phillips, Robert (ed.). Aesthetic Theories of India. University of Hawaii Press.
- Harre, Rom; Parrott, W. Gerrod, eds. (1996). The Emotions: Social, Cultural and Biological Dimensions. Sage.
- Hawton, Keith; Saunders, Kate E. A.; O'Connor, Rory C. (2012-06-23). "Self-harm and suicide in adolescents". The Lancet. 379 (9834): 2373–2382. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60322-5. ISSN 0140-6736. PMID 22726518. S2CID 151486181.
- Hochschild, Arlie (1979). "Emotion Work, Feeling Rules, and Social Structure" (PDF). American Journal of Sociology. 85 (3): 551–575. doi:10.1086/227049. S2CID 143485249.
- Hochschild, Arlie Russell (2003) [1983]. The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-93041-4.
- Horne, Outi; Csipke, Emese (2009). "From Feeling Too Little and Too Much, to Feeling More and Less? A Nonparadoxical Theory of the Functions of Self-Harm". Qualitative Health Research. 19 (5): 655–667. doi:10.1177/1049732309334249. PMID 19380501. S2CID 40361244. (subscription required)
- Izard, Carroll E. (1977). Human Emotions. Springer. ISBN 978-0-306-30986-1.
- Jung, Carl Gustav (1971) [1921]. Psychological Types. Princeton University Press.
- James, William (1884). "What is an Emotion?". Mind. 9 (34): 188–205.
- Lazarus, Richard S. (1982). "Thoughts on the Relations Between Emotion and Cognition". American Psychologist. 37 (9): 1019–1024. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.37.9.1019.
- Lewis, Michael; Haviland-Jones, Jeannette M.; Barrett, Lisa Feldman, eds. (2016). Handbook of Emotions (4th ed.). Guilford Press.
- Mathias, Manon; Moore, Alison M., eds. (2018). Gut Feeling and Digestive Health in Nineteenth-Century Literature, History and Culture. New York: Palgrave. ISBN 978-3-030-01857-3.
- Maslow, Abraham H. (1970). Motivation andPpersonality (2 ed.). New York: Harper & Row. ISBN 0-06-044241-7. OCLC 89585.
- Nussbaum, Martha C. (2001). Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge University Press.
- Panksepp, Jaak (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford University Press.
- Scheler, Max (1973) [1916]. Formalism in Ethics and Non-Formal Ethics of Values. Northwestern University Press.
- Sharp, Daryl (1987). Personality Types: Jung's Model of Typology. Inner City Books.
- Tsoknyi Rinpoche (1998). Carefree Dignity: Discourses on Training in the Nature of Mind. Rangjung Yeshe Publications.
- Wasserman, T.; Wasserman, L. (2020). "Motivation: State, Trait, or Both". Motivation, Effort, and the Neural Network Model. pp. 93–101. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-58724-6_8. ISBN 978-3-030-58724-6. S2CID 229258237.
- Wood, Stacy L.; Bettman, James R. (2007-07-01). "Predicting Happiness: How Normative Feeling Rules Influence (and Even Reverse) Durability Bias". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 17 (3): 188–201. doi:10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70028-1.
- Wundt, Wilhelm (1897). Outlines of Psychology. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.
- Zajonc, Robert (1980). "Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences". American Psychologist. 35 (2): 151–175. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151.
Further reading
- Ngai, Sianne (2005). Ugly Feelings. Harvard University Press.
- Terada, Rei (2001). Feeling in Theory: Emotion after the "Death of the Subject". Harvard University Press.
External links

The dictionary definition of feeling at Wiktionary
wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library, article, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games, mobile, phone, android, ios, apple, mobile phone, samsung, iphone, xiomi, xiaomi, redmi, honor, oppo, nokia, sonya, mi, pc, web, computer
This article may require cleanup to meet Wikipedia s quality standards The specific problem is Some sections remain fragmented or weakly integrated e g Sensation Needs Perception Further editing is needed to improve cohesion Please help improve this article if you can March 2025 Learn how and when to remove this message According to the APA Dictionary of Psychology a feeling is a self contained phenomenal experience feelings are subjective evaluative and independent of the sensations thoughts or images evoking them The term feeling is closely related to but not the same as emotion Feeling may for instance refer to the conscious subjective experience of emotions The study of subjective experiences is called phenomenology Psychotherapy generally involves a therapist helping a client understand articulate and learn to effectively regulate the client s own feelings and ultimately to take responsibility for the client s experience of the world Feelings are sometimes held to be characteristic of embodied consciousness The English noun feelings may generally refer to any degree of subjectivity in perception or sensation However feelings often refer to an individual sense of well being perhaps of wholeness safety or being loved Feelings have a semantic field extending from the individual and spiritual to the social and political The word feeling may refer to any of a number of psychological characteristics of experience or even to reflect the entire inner life of the individual see mood As self contained phenomenal experiences evoked by sensations and perceptions feelings can strongly influence the character of a person s subjective reality Feelings can sometimes harbor bias or otherwise distort veridical perception in particular through projection wishful thinking among many other such effects Feeling may also describe the senses such as the physical sensation of touch Definitions and distinctionsIn psychology and philosophy feeling is commonly defined as the subjective experience of emotion or sensation Although the terms feeling emotion affect and mood are sometimes used interchangeably in everyday language they have distinct meanings in academic contexts According to psychologist Carroll Izard feelings are best understood as the conscious experience of emotion arising when an affective state reaches awareness William James similarly proposed that feelings result from the perception of bodily changes in response to external stimuli thus forming part of the emotional process More recently affective neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp emphasized the role of subcortical brain systems in generating core affects that underlie both feelings and emotions Lisa Feldman Barrett argues that feelings are constructed mental representations emerging from the brain s interpretation of interoceptive signals combined with prior experience and cultural concepts In this view feelings are not innate discrete entities but are formed through predictive processes In philosophical psychology particularly in the work of Carl Jung feeling is considered one of the four primary functions of consciousness alongside thinking sensation and intuition Unlike emotions which are often reactive Jung defined feeling as a rational function that evaluates and assigns value Feeling also differs from sensation while sensation refers to raw sensory input such as touch heat or pain feelings involve evaluative or affective judgements about those sensations or experiences Similarly moods are typically more diffuse and long lasting affective states while feelings tend to be more transient and directly tied to particular events or thoughts These distinctions are foundational in fields such as affective science philosophy of mind and cognitive psychology where the term feeling plays a central role in understanding consciousness subjectivity and emotional life Neuroscientist Antonio Damasio distinguishes between emotions and feelings Emotions are mental images i e representing either internal or external states of reality and the bodily changes accompanying them whereas feelings are the perception of bodily changes In other words emotions contain a subjective element and a third person observable element whereas feelings are subjective and private 10 Historical and philosophical viewsThe English word feeling derives from Old English felan meaning to touch or perceive through the senses and later acquired the meaning of internal emotional experience Early philosophical and psychological approaches to feeling laid the foundation for later distinctions between affect emotion and cognition These perspectives treated feeling not merely as emotional fluctuation but as a central dimension of human experience evaluative thought and even moral judgement The systematic study of affect and feeling gefuhl in psychology began in the late 19th century with the work of Wilhelm Wundt often considered the founder of experimental psychology Wundt proposed that affective experience could be described along three dimensions pleasant unpleasant arousing subduing and strain relaxation These affective dimensions laid the groundwork for later theories of emotional valence and arousal A decade later William James proposed a physiological theory of emotion in which feelings are the perception of bodily changes caused by external stimuli In his classic 1884 essay he wrote we feel sorry because we cry angry because we strike afraid because we tremble arguing that feeling follows bodily reaction rather than preceding it In the early 20th century Carl Jung developed a typology in which feeling was one of the four fundamental functions of consciousness alongside thinking sensation and intuition Unlike emotion which he considered reactive and affective Jung defined feeling as a rational function that judges and evaluates rather than perceives or reacts In this view feeling can be used to assign value or make decisions independent of sensory experience Meanwhile in phenomenological philosophy Max Scheler emphasized that feeling is a unique mode of access to values Rather than viewing feelings as subjective or irrational Scheler argued that they are intentional acts that disclose the worth of things what he called value feelings wertgefuhle This idea positioned feeling not only as affective but also as epistemological Contemporary philosopher Martha Nussbaum has continued the philosophical development of feeling by arguing that emotions are a form of evaluative judgement Drawing from classical philosophy she suggests that emotions are not opposed to rationality but are instead shaped by beliefs about what is valuable and significant In her account feelings are deeply intertwined with ethical reasoning and human flourishing Cross cultural and contemplative viewsConceptions of feeling vary widely across cultures and philosophical traditions In many non Western frameworks feeling is not merely a passive or internal state but a central mode of perceiving valuing and engaging with the world In Buddhist psychology particularly in the Abhidharma and Mahayana traditions feelings Sanskrit vedana are classified into three primary types pleasant unpleasant and neutral These are not emotions per se but rather the basic hedonic tone that arises with every moment of experience The recognition and mindfulness of feeling tones is foundational in the Satipatthana system of meditation particularly in the practice of vedananupassana the contemplation of feelings as transient phenomena In Tibetan Buddhist systems these basic feeling tones are further elaborated into a structured typology known as the 51 mental factors which include both innate and cultivated emotional and cognitive states Feeling in this context is interwoven with attentional processes ethical evaluation and the potential for insight These frameworks regard feeling as a dynamic event in the continuum of mind with implications for both enlightenment and suffering Hindu philosophy particularly in the context of rasa theory offers another model in which feelings are treated not as inner states alone but as aesthetic emotional essences rasa that are evoked and shared through performance poetry and religious experience Classical Indian aesthetics identifies nine primary rasas such as love sṛṅgara sorrow karuṇa and wonder adbhuta each associated with a specific emotional flavor that is both individually felt and socially mediated Scientific theories and modelsA number of experiments have been conducted in the study of social and psychological affective preferences i e what people like or dislike Specific research has been done on preferences attitudes impression formation and decision making This research contrasts findings with recognition memory old new judgments allowing researchers to demonstrate reliable distinctions between the two Affect based judgments and cognitive processes have been examined with noted differences indicated Some argue affect and cognition are under the control of separate and partially independent systems that can influence each other in a variety of ways Both affect and cognition may constitute independent sources of effects within systems of information processing Others suggest emotion is a result of an anticipated experienced or imagined outcome of an adaptational transaction between organism and environment therefore cognitive appraisal processes are keys to the development and expression of an emotion This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it March 2025 SensationsSensation occurs when sense organs collect various stimuli such as a sound or smell for transduction meaning transformation into a form that can be understood by the nervous system Interoception Gut Examples of six basic emotions A gut feeling or gut reaction is a visceral emotional reaction to something It may be negative such as a feeling of uneasiness or positive such as a feeling of trust Gut feelings are generally regarded as not modulated by conscious thought but sometimes as a feature of intuition rather than rationality The idea that emotions are experienced in the gut has a long historical legacy and many nineteenth century doctors considered the origins of mental illness to derive from the intestines 22 The phrase gut feeling may also be used as a shorthand term for an individual s common sense perception of what is considered the right thing to do such as helping an injured passerby avoiding dark alleys and generally acting in accordance with instinctive feelings about a given situation It can also refer to simple common knowledge phrases which are true no matter when said such as Fire is hot or to ideas that an individual intuitively regards as true see truthiness for examples Heart The heart has a collection of ganglia that is called the intrinsic cardiac nervous system The feelings of affiliation love attachment anger hurt are usually associated with the heart especially the feeling of love NeedsA need is something required to sustain a healthy life e g air water food A need deficiency causes a clear adverse outcome a dysfunction or death Abraham H Maslow pointed out that satisfying i e gratification of a need is just as important as deprivation i e motivation to satisfy for it releases the focus of the satisfied need to other emergent needs Motivation Motivation is what explains why people or animals initiate continue or terminate a certain behavior at a particular time Motivational states are commonly understood as forces acting within the agent that create a disposition to engage in goal directed behavior It is often held that different mental states compete with each other and that only the strongest state determines behavior Valence Valence tells organisms e g humans how well or how bad an organism is doing in relation to the environment for meeting the organism s needs PerceptionFeelings of certainty The way that we see other people express their emotions or feelings determines how we respond The way an individual responds to a situation is based on feeling rules If an individual is uninformed about a situation the way they respond would be in a completely different demeanor than if they were informed about a situation For example if a tragic event had occurred and they had knowledge of it their response would be sympathetic to that situation If they had no knowledge of the situation then their response may be indifference A lack of knowledge or information about an event can shape the way an individual sees things and the way they respond Timothy D Wilson a psychology professor tested this theory of the feeling of uncertainty along with his colleague Yoav Bar Anan a social psychologist Wilson and Bar Anan found that the more uncertain or unclear an individual is about a situation the more invested they are Since an individual does not know the background or the ending of a story they are constantly replaying an event in their mind which is causing them to have mixed feelings of happiness sadness excitement and et cetera If there is any difference between feelings and emotions the feeling of uncertainty is less sure than the emotion of ambivalence the former is precarious the latter is not yet acted upon or decided upon The neurologist Robert Burton writes in his book On Being Certain that feelings of certainty may stem from involuntary mental sensations much like emotions or perceptual recognition another example might be the tip of the tongue phenomenon Individuals in society want to know every detail about something in hopes to maximize the feeling for that moment but Wilson found that feeling uncertain can lead to something being more enjoyable because it has a sense of mystery In fact the feeling of not knowing can lead them to constantly think and feel about what could have been Sense of agency and sense of ownership This section is empty You can help by adding to it March 2025 Feelings about feelingsSensitive sculpture by M Blay c 1910 Individuals in society predict that something will give them a certain desired outcome or feeling Indulging in what one might have thought would ve made them happy or excited might only cause a temporary thrill or it might result in the opposite of what was expected and wanted Events and experiences are done and relived to satisfy one s feelings citation needed Details and information about the past are used to make decisions as past experiences of feelings tend to influence current decision making how people will feel in the future and if they want to feel that way again Gilbert and Wilson conducted a study to show how pleased a person would feel if they purchased flowers for themselves for no specific reason birthday anniversary or promotion etc and how long they thought that feeling would last People who had no experience of purchasing flowers for themselves and those who had experienced buying flowers for themselves were tested Results showed that those who had purchased flowers in the past for themselves felt happier and that feeling lasted longer for them than for a person who had never experienced purchasing flowers for themselves Arlie Russell Hochschild a sociologist depicted two accounts of emotion The organismic emotion is the outburst of emotions and feelings In organismic emotion emotions feelings are instantly expressed Social and other factors do not influence how the emotion is perceived so these factors have no control on how or if the emotion is suppressed or expressed In interactive emotion emotions and feelings are controlled The individual is constantly considering how to react or what to suppress In interactive emotion unlike in organismic emotion the individual is aware of their decision on how they feel and how they show it Erving Goffman a sociologist and writer compared how actors withheld their emotions to the everyday individual Like actors individuals can control how emotions are expressed but they cannot control their inner emotions or feelings Inner feelings can only be suppressed in order to achieve the expression one wants people to see on the outside Goffman explains that emotions and emotional experience are an ongoing thing that an individual is consciously and actively working through Individuals want to conform to society with their inner and outer feelings Anger happiness joy stress and excitement are some of the feelings that can be experienced in life In response to these emotions our bodies react as well For example nervousness can lead to the sensation of having knots in the stomach or butterflies in the stomach Self harm Negative feelings can lead to harm When an individual is dealing with an overwhelming amount of stress and problems in their lives there is the possibility that they might consider self harm When one is in a good state of feeling they never want it to end conversely when someone is in a bad state of mind they want that feeling to disappear Inflicting harm or pain to oneself is sometimes the answer for many individuals because they want something to keep their mind off the real problem These individuals cut stab and starve themselves in an effort to feel something other than what they currently feel as they believe the pain to be not as bad as their actual problem Distraction is not the only reason why many individuals choose to inflict self harm Some people inflict self harm to punish themselves for feeling a certain way Other psychological factors could be low self esteem the need to be perfect or social anxiety See alsoAffective science Alexithymia Cognitive neuroscience Hard problem of consciousness Mind body problem QualiaNotesIn German psychology the term gefuhl meaning feeling has long been used to refer to subjective emotional experience and it continues to play a role in German language affective science and phenomenology References APA Dictionary of Psychology dictionary apa org Retrieved 2022 03 24 VandenBos Gary 2006 APA Dictionary of Psychology Washington DC American Psychological Association Solms Mark 2021 The hidden spring a journey to the source of consciousness London ISBN 978 1 78816 283 8 OCLC 1190847187 a href wiki Template Cite book title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link Izard 1977 James 1884 Panksepp 1998 Barrett 2017 Jung 1971 Sharp 1987 Panksepp 1998 Ekkekakis 2013 Lewis Haviland Jones amp Barrett 2016 Damasio 1994 p page needed Damasio 1999 p page needed Harre amp Parrott 1996 Wundt 1897 Sharp 1987 Scheler 1973 Nussbaum 2001 Tsoknyi Rinpoche 1998 Analayo 2003 Tsoknyi Rinpoche 1998 Berzin 2006 Gerow 1982 Zajonc 1980 Lazarus 1982 Mathias amp Moore 2018 p page needed Fedele amp Brand 2020 Maslow 1970 p 38 Wasserman amp Wasserman 2020 Hochschild 2003 Bar Anan Wilson amp Gilbert 2009 Burton 2008 Horne amp Csipke 2009 Wood amp Bettman 2007 Hochschild 1979 Hawton Saunders amp O Connor 2012 Works cited Analayo 2003 Satipaṭṭhana The Direct Path to Realization Windhorse Publications Bar Anan Y Wilson T D Gilbert D T 2009 The feeling of uncertainty intensifies affective reactions Emotion 9 1 123 7 doi 10 1037 a0014607 PMID 19186925 S2CID 10179263 Barrett Lisa Feldman 2017 How Emotions Are Made The Secret Life of the Brain Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Berzin Alexander 2006 Primary Minds and the 51 Mental Factors Study Buddhism Retrieved 2025 03 31 Burton R A 2008 On Being Certain Believing You Are Right Even When You re Not St Martin s Press ISBN 978 0 312 35920 1 Damasio Antonio 1994 Descartes Error Emotion Reason and the Human Brain New York Putnam ISBN 0 399 13894 3 Damasio Antonio 1999 The Feeling of what Happens Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness New York Harcourt Brace ISBN 978 0 15 601075 7 Ekkekakis Panteleimon 2013 The Measurement of Affect Mood and Emotion A Guide for Health Behavioral Research Cambridge University Press Fedele Laura Brand Thomas December 2020 The Intrinsic Cardiac Nervous System and Its Role in Cardiac Pacemaking and Conduction Journal of Cardiovascular Development and Disease 7 4 54 doi 10 3390 jcdd7040054 PMC 7712215 PMID 33255284 Gerow Edwin 1982 Rasa as a Category of Literary Criticism In Phillips Robert ed Aesthetic Theories of India University of Hawaii Press Harre Rom Parrott W Gerrod eds 1996 The Emotions Social Cultural and Biological Dimensions Sage Hawton Keith Saunders Kate E A O Connor Rory C 2012 06 23 Self harm and suicide in adolescents The Lancet 379 9834 2373 2382 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 12 60322 5 ISSN 0140 6736 PMID 22726518 S2CID 151486181 Hochschild Arlie 1979 Emotion Work Feeling Rules and Social Structure PDF American Journal of Sociology 85 3 551 575 doi 10 1086 227049 S2CID 143485249 Hochschild Arlie Russell 2003 1983 The Managed Heart Commercialization of Human Feeling University of California Press ISBN 978 0 520 93041 4 Horne Outi Csipke Emese 2009 From Feeling Too Little and Too Much to Feeling More and Less A Nonparadoxical Theory of the Functions of Self Harm Qualitative Health Research 19 5 655 667 doi 10 1177 1049732309334249 PMID 19380501 S2CID 40361244 subscription required Izard Carroll E 1977 Human Emotions Springer ISBN 978 0 306 30986 1 Jung Carl Gustav 1971 1921 Psychological Types Princeton University Press James William 1884 What is an Emotion Mind 9 34 188 205 Lazarus Richard S 1982 Thoughts on the Relations Between Emotion and Cognition American Psychologist 37 9 1019 1024 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 37 9 1019 Lewis Michael Haviland Jones Jeannette M Barrett Lisa Feldman eds 2016 Handbook of Emotions 4th ed Guilford Press Mathias Manon Moore Alison M eds 2018 Gut Feeling and Digestive Health in Nineteenth Century Literature History and Culture New York Palgrave ISBN 978 3 030 01857 3 Maslow Abraham H 1970 Motivation andPpersonality 2 ed New York Harper amp Row ISBN 0 06 044241 7 OCLC 89585 Nussbaum Martha C 2001 Upheavals of Thought The Intelligence of Emotions Cambridge University Press Panksepp Jaak 1998 Affective Neuroscience The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions Oxford University Press Scheler Max 1973 1916 Formalism in Ethics and Non Formal Ethics of Values Northwestern University Press Sharp Daryl 1987 Personality Types Jung s Model of Typology Inner City Books Tsoknyi Rinpoche 1998 Carefree Dignity Discourses on Training in the Nature of Mind Rangjung Yeshe Publications Wasserman T Wasserman L 2020 Motivation State Trait or Both Motivation Effort and the Neural Network Model pp 93 101 doi 10 1007 978 3 030 58724 6 8 ISBN 978 3 030 58724 6 S2CID 229258237 Wood Stacy L Bettman James R 2007 07 01 Predicting Happiness How Normative Feeling Rules Influence and Even Reverse Durability Bias Journal of Consumer Psychology 17 3 188 201 doi 10 1016 S1057 7408 07 70028 1 Wundt Wilhelm 1897 Outlines of Psychology Leipzig Wilhelm Engelmann Zajonc Robert 1980 Feeling and thinking Preferences need no inferences American Psychologist 35 2 151 175 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 35 2 151 Further readingNgai Sianne 2005 Ugly Feelings Harvard University Press Terada Rei 2001 Feeling in Theory Emotion after the Death of the Subject Harvard University Press External linksWikisource has the text of the 1921 Collier s Encyclopedia article Feeling The dictionary definition of feeling at Wiktionary